advertisement
When considering the application of clinical research to our patients, it is our personal preference to use the common sense (aka clinical epidemiology) concepts of NNT (number needed to treat), Relative Risk (RR) and Relative Risk Reduction (RRR).
The NNT reflects the number of patients we need to treat in order to obtain one benefit or desirable result. It is calculated from the absolute risks in the treatment and control groups.1 All else being equal, the lower the NNT, the better. The NNT allows us to identify high-risk or high-response sub-groups of patients who have a small NNT and stand the most to gain from the intervention in question. It also allows us to compare interventions. The relative risk (RR) tells us the risk of an outcome in one group with the risk factor (eg increased IOP) compared to the risk of an outcome in a group without the risk factor (eg normal IOP).2 A derivation of the RR, the relative risk reduction (RRR) tells us the proportion of the baseline risk that can potentially be removed by treating the risk factor in question. While we consider all the above measures in the interpretation of data, NNT is our preferred method of translating clinical research to patient care. Relative Risk Reduction (RRR) and excess risk (that we will encounter) are especially useful when intervention effects are small.
In the Early Manifest Glaucoma Treatment Study (EMGT), over a 5- year period 62% of untreated subjects (absolute risk in controls) progressed, compared to 45% in the treated group (absolute risk in the treated group).3 The RR of progression in the EMGT was therefore (62/45) = 1.38. How do we interpret this RR? Or, in other words, what is a 'good' RR? Our epidemiologist friend tells us that RR's below 2 are rarely significant. According to the author of our clinical Bible,1 depending on the type of study RR's of 3-4 are likely significant, and 20 is probably high enough to attribute causality. As an example, an RR of 5.7, with confidence intervals of 1.9-17.1 ascribed to diurnal variation is impressive on its own.4 It doesn't require any manipulation to make it look more impressive. Especially since the lower end of the confidence interval (CI; 1.9-17.1) is around the ball park figure, we would be interested anyway.
What if the relative risk is lower than what would usually be considered
significant? Like 1.38 for EMGT? A good way to present a small RR,
especially to decision makers, is to use the Relative Risk Reduction:
Absolute Risk in the control group - Absolute risk in the treated group | × 100 | Absolute Risk in the control group
For the EMGT: (62-45/62) × 100 = 27.42%. This is more easily calculated
as RR-1/RR, and provides the same result. There is little doubt about the role of raised IOP in the causation and
progression of glaucoma. However, results from some recent studies have
been presented in an isolated manner that might encourage very aggressive
lowering of the IOP with possibly detrimental effects. A relative risk of 1.1 is small (large numbers can make anything statistically significant). If the relative risk is small, (like 1.1), one way to make it look attractive as we learnt is the RRR. The RRR for a relative risk of 1.1 (RR-1/RR) is 9%. There is another way to make this look even more attractive or 'sellable', so to speak. Instead of the relative risk reduction, we can use 'excess' risk. The formula for excess risk is RR-1 expressed as a percentage = 1.1-1 = 10%. This derivation (with other assumptions) has probably lead to the '10% reduction per mmHg reduction' interpretation. While it is the truth, the statement is perhaps best interpreted keeping the overall picture in mind. We personally feel that a combination of all the measures, absolute risk, relative risk, relative risk reduction and of course our favorite, the NNT, provides far more useful and clinically useable information than the RR (or RRR) alone. References
|